In the last month, I’ve had at least three conversations with researchers on Twitter about why they haven’t published in Open Access journals. The obstacle in each case was, “I can’t pay the fees.”
When I asked them if they knew that at least some open access journals (notably PLOS ONE) would consider waiving the fee. I pointed them to this article as evidence. I now have another concrete example, courtesy of Proteins and Wave Functions.
“You can do that?” was sort of the reaction from a couple of people, who were either unaware that there was such a thing, or that it applied to them. This is understandable, given that waivers in many academic societies are given for people in, say, “developing nations.” (One person asked, but the journal in question didn’t have a fee waiver that policy.)
“People don’t have enough information!” is overused in explaining other people’s behaviour, but maybe that explanation has some traction in this case. Open Access journals and supporters need to do a much better job of communicating fee waiver policies. People who want to publish Open Access believe that they are excluded, when they may not be.
Speaking of costs, there is new article in Nature that covers the cost effectiveness of Open Access journals online resource. It’s accompanied by this graph:
The article says there isn’t much influence between the author fees and the “influence” of the journal, but I wonder how much of that lack of correlation might be due to the large number of journals with little or no cost on the left hand side. The correlation from $100 to $2,000 looks... surprisingly good.
What I also find interesting about this graph is that you can see a few pricing “sweet spots” in the graph: $0, $800, $1,500, and about $1,800. I wonder what causing those particular prices to act as “attractors.” Around the $1,800 mark, it appears that many of those journals are from the same publisher, BioMed Central? The other prices, though, are harder to suss out. Are publishers are pricing their fees to be comparable to each other? Or maybe all those prices converge because that’s about the price of publishing a paper for those journals? Don’t know.
External links
What does it cost to publish a Gold Open Access article?
The science 1%ers pumping Open Access are dangerously out of touch
31 January 2013
Science Online 2013, day 1: In which I wonder if this was all a huge mistake
I suspected I was in trouble when I walked into the airport and there was nobody else in line for check in.
Then, airport security mistook me for a chemist. Oh, the indignity.
Sure enough, my 9:10 am flight that I'd come for no longer existed. I was now on a 10:45 am flight. Thanks for keeping me informed, United Airlines. I will say in your favour, United, that your flight attendant on UA 3849 (my 10:45 flight) deserves a raise. She was awesome and funny, and got applause from the cabin twice.
I thought I got to the airport in time to catch my flight to Raleigh, but I miscalculated. The flight depart had been moved up about 20 minutes, and I mistook departure time for boarding time, didn't ask if the flight had started to board when I got to the gate, didn't realize I missed all the boarding calls, and consequently, missed my flight. The next United flight was full, but I managed to get transferred over to another flight on US Airways.
While I waited, the news was showing the wreckage from tornadoes that were caused by the storm that was expected to be hitting Raleigh more or less the time I was supposed to arrive there.
All this put more in a foul mood. I was mad at myself for missing a flight I might have been able to make, and the schedule changes. I seriously started questioning whether conferences were worth it to me any more.
As I flew over the clouds, I wondered if any of them were the storm clouds that were causing so much grief today. I wondered what a tornado cloud looked like from above.
The new flight was not direct to Raleigh. It touched down in Charlotte first, so I had enough time for a bathroom break, but there was no food I wanted to eat nearby. I spotted Emily Willingham (Double X Science), which was nice, and arranged to split a cab after we got off the plane.
Getting off the plane would take some time, however. Just as we were boarding, the weather that I'd seen forecast days before, and watched on the news, finally caught up with us. Heavy, heavy rain that was coming down in buckets like when then turn on fire hoses to make in rain in movies because you need huge amounts of water for it to show up on film. We got on to the runway, and then spent an hour there, because outgoing eastbound flights were delayed until the storm front cleared out a bit.
I finally arrived in Raleigh about 10 pm, something like six hours after I was supposed to have arrived. In fact, I think my entire trip was supposed to last just six hours. I walked into the hotel lobby to check in, heard what can only be described as a din coming form the bar. Science Online was in full swing, all right. I was still feeling foul and wanting sleep, so I went into my room to hide and try to reset my dial to the green zone.
On the plus side, when I checked my email... manuscript accepted!
Then, airport security mistook me for a chemist. Oh, the indignity.
Sure enough, my 9:10 am flight that I'd come for no longer existed. I was now on a 10:45 am flight. Thanks for keeping me informed, United Airlines. I will say in your favour, United, that your flight attendant on UA 3849 (my 10:45 flight) deserves a raise. She was awesome and funny, and got applause from the cabin twice.
I thought I got to the airport in time to catch my flight to Raleigh, but I miscalculated. The flight depart had been moved up about 20 minutes, and I mistook departure time for boarding time, didn't ask if the flight had started to board when I got to the gate, didn't realize I missed all the boarding calls, and consequently, missed my flight. The next United flight was full, but I managed to get transferred over to another flight on US Airways.
While I waited, the news was showing the wreckage from tornadoes that were caused by the storm that was expected to be hitting Raleigh more or less the time I was supposed to arrive there.
All this put more in a foul mood. I was mad at myself for missing a flight I might have been able to make, and the schedule changes. I seriously started questioning whether conferences were worth it to me any more.
As I flew over the clouds, I wondered if any of them were the storm clouds that were causing so much grief today. I wondered what a tornado cloud looked like from above.
The new flight was not direct to Raleigh. It touched down in Charlotte first, so I had enough time for a bathroom break, but there was no food I wanted to eat nearby. I spotted Emily Willingham (Double X Science), which was nice, and arranged to split a cab after we got off the plane.
Getting off the plane would take some time, however. Just as we were boarding, the weather that I'd seen forecast days before, and watched on the news, finally caught up with us. Heavy, heavy rain that was coming down in buckets like when then turn on fire hoses to make in rain in movies because you need huge amounts of water for it to show up on film. We got on to the runway, and then spent an hour there, because outgoing eastbound flights were delayed until the storm front cleared out a bit.
I finally arrived in Raleigh about 10 pm, something like six hours after I was supposed to have arrived. In fact, I think my entire trip was supposed to last just six hours. I walked into the hotel lobby to check in, heard what can only be described as a din coming form the bar. Science Online was in full swing, all right. I was still feeling foul and wanting sleep, so I went into my room to hide and try to reset my dial to the green zone.
On the plus side, when I checked my email... manuscript accepted!
30 January 2013
Going analog at a digital conference: #scio13
When I’m on the floor of Science Online 2013 in the next few days, this and a pen is the only thing I plan to have with me.
I’ll leaving my smart phone and tablet in the hotel room. I won’t be live tweeting. I may do a couple of end of day summaries here on NeuroDojo.
At other conferences, I am usually one of very few online. I have learned that people enjoy getting conference updates, so I do it partly out of a sense of service. But at Science Online, instead of being in a very tiny minority, it’s going to be 449 other people like me. This is probably going to be themost documented conference I will ever attend.
The point of a conference is to do something different. I interact with many of these people online regularly. Why would I do the same when I have them right here in front of me?
I don’t want to get to the conference and feel like Buzz Aldrin after Apollo 11. Aldrin watching a video of the press coverage of the Moon landing while in quarantine after splashdown, turned to Neil Armstrong, and said:
At Science Online, I want to be, as actors say, “in the moment.” Fully present, paying attention, and listening.
Update, 6 February 2013: Thought I was kidding about everything being documented at Science Online?
From here.
I’ll leaving my smart phone and tablet in the hotel room. I won’t be live tweeting. I may do a couple of end of day summaries here on NeuroDojo.
At other conferences, I am usually one of very few online. I have learned that people enjoy getting conference updates, so I do it partly out of a sense of service. But at Science Online, instead of being in a very tiny minority, it’s going to be 449 other people like me. This is probably going to be themost documented conference I will ever attend.
The point of a conference is to do something different. I interact with many of these people online regularly. Why would I do the same when I have them right here in front of me?
I don’t want to get to the conference and feel like Buzz Aldrin after Apollo 11. Aldrin watching a video of the press coverage of the Moon landing while in quarantine after splashdown, turned to Neil Armstrong, and said:
Hey, we missed the whole thing.
At Science Online, I want to be, as actors say, “in the moment.” Fully present, paying attention, and listening.
Update, 6 February 2013: Thought I was kidding about everything being documented at Science Online?
From here.
29 January 2013
Tuesday Crustie: The deadly mantis (shrimp)
This is just one of an amazing set of photos by Jason Isley. Jason decided to take miniatures to the sea floor, and have them interacting with the sea life he found there. Click any to enlarge...
Not photoshopped, just straight photography.
From The Daily Mail. Hat tip to Brian Switek on Twitter.
28 January 2013
Whales: big enough, too big, or bigger than big?
This picture can’t do them justice. No picture can.
That’s because this is a picture of a blue whale, the largest animal to live on this planet. Ever.
Goodness knows, people try to show you the size. They put up mounts of blue whale skeletons in museums, or life sized models. There’s a very cool online animation that shows images from the blue whale full sized, on your computer screen, as has it drift by lazily. But I suspect that even these clever things can’t do the trick of conveying what the size of the living, breathing animal must be.
But while the blue whale has the undisputed title of being the biggest, whales, dolphins, and their brethren in general are all very big compared to most mammals. In a new paper, Clauset tests a model that tries to explain why whales might so big.
Normally, when I think of limits to size, I think of biomechanical and physical constraints. “In a big animal, can you make the bone thick enough to move without breaking?”, for instance. This is a common sort of explanation for why you can’t make giant insects like in the old 1950s monster movies:
Strength increases as you make muscles bigger, but strength doesn’t increase as fast as mass does.
What’s interesting about Clauset’s approach is that he explains the sizes of whales without using too many of these sorts of arguments. He does invoke the physics to explain the limits to small sizes. Mammals (and birds) can only be so small because of how they regulate their body temperature. If you’re too small, you cannot eat enough food to make up for the heat flow away from your body.
This thermoregulation problem explains why there are no cat-sized dolphins that you keep in a backyard pool, or hamster-sized porpoises you can keep in a home aquarium. The smallest cetacean is the La Plata dolphin, which are around 35-50 kg as adults. Although the babies are pretty squee-worthy:
Clauset ignores all the details of biomechanical and physical and energetic limitations by rolling all that into “extinction risk.” Can make bones strong enough? Can’t eat enough food? All of those mean that those big species are more likely to go extinct.
Clauset assumes a species can either get bigger or smaller over evolutionary time, although Clauset assumes there are some fitness advantages to being bigger. You have a hard limit on how small you can get set by your ability to themoregulate. The limit to how big you can get is a soft limit set by the likelihood your lineage will go extinct. With only these facts, Clauset’s model fits the size distribution of cetaceans extremely well. Presumably, the same model could be used for terrestrial mammals or birds.
Even the massive blue whale, Clauset says, is not particularly unlikely according to his model. Clauset draws out the line from his model and suggests that it might be possible to have a whale species that is over three times bigger than blue whales; 3.7 times, to be exact. Clauset notes that such a massive whale could not just be the blue whale scaled up. To be bigger than the blues, a new whale species might have to evolve some innovation that would allow them to forage more efficiently than the blue whale’s lunge feeding.
The notion that even the blue what could be dwarfed by another sea creature is an awe inspiring thought.
Reference
Clauset A. 2013. How large should whales be? PLOS ONE 8(1): e53967. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053967
Blue whale photo by Seabass London on Flickr; used under a Creative Commons license. La Plata dolphin from Washington Post.
That’s because this is a picture of a blue whale, the largest animal to live on this planet. Ever.
Goodness knows, people try to show you the size. They put up mounts of blue whale skeletons in museums, or life sized models. There’s a very cool online animation that shows images from the blue whale full sized, on your computer screen, as has it drift by lazily. But I suspect that even these clever things can’t do the trick of conveying what the size of the living, breathing animal must be.
But while the blue whale has the undisputed title of being the biggest, whales, dolphins, and their brethren in general are all very big compared to most mammals. In a new paper, Clauset tests a model that tries to explain why whales might so big.
Normally, when I think of limits to size, I think of biomechanical and physical constraints. “In a big animal, can you make the bone thick enough to move without breaking?”, for instance. This is a common sort of explanation for why you can’t make giant insects like in the old 1950s monster movies:
Strength increases as you make muscles bigger, but strength doesn’t increase as fast as mass does.
What’s interesting about Clauset’s approach is that he explains the sizes of whales without using too many of these sorts of arguments. He does invoke the physics to explain the limits to small sizes. Mammals (and birds) can only be so small because of how they regulate their body temperature. If you’re too small, you cannot eat enough food to make up for the heat flow away from your body.
This thermoregulation problem explains why there are no cat-sized dolphins that you keep in a backyard pool, or hamster-sized porpoises you can keep in a home aquarium. The smallest cetacean is the La Plata dolphin, which are around 35-50 kg as adults. Although the babies are pretty squee-worthy:
Clauset ignores all the details of biomechanical and physical and energetic limitations by rolling all that into “extinction risk.” Can make bones strong enough? Can’t eat enough food? All of those mean that those big species are more likely to go extinct.
Clauset assumes a species can either get bigger or smaller over evolutionary time, although Clauset assumes there are some fitness advantages to being bigger. You have a hard limit on how small you can get set by your ability to themoregulate. The limit to how big you can get is a soft limit set by the likelihood your lineage will go extinct. With only these facts, Clauset’s model fits the size distribution of cetaceans extremely well. Presumably, the same model could be used for terrestrial mammals or birds.
Even the massive blue whale, Clauset says, is not particularly unlikely according to his model. Clauset draws out the line from his model and suggests that it might be possible to have a whale species that is over three times bigger than blue whales; 3.7 times, to be exact. Clauset notes that such a massive whale could not just be the blue whale scaled up. To be bigger than the blues, a new whale species might have to evolve some innovation that would allow them to forage more efficiently than the blue whale’s lunge feeding.
The notion that even the blue what could be dwarfed by another sea creature is an awe inspiring thought.
Reference
Clauset A. 2013. How large should whales be? PLOS ONE 8(1): e53967. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0053967
Blue whale photo by Seabass London on Flickr; used under a Creative Commons license. La Plata dolphin from Washington Post.
Amazons and Goliaths, my #SciFund expedition blog, now available!
Back in November, when I went on a trip supported by two rounds of #SciFund money, I made a “virtual expedition” blog for my supporters.
I called it “Amazons and Goliaths” after the titles of my two #SciFund projects: Doctor Zen and the Amazon Crayfish Civilization and Beach of the Goliath Crabs. To read it in chronological order (recommended), start at the bottom of this page and read backwards. Pretend you’re reading manga.
The blog has been mostly quiet since November, but I’m starting to do some of the analysis of the data I got, and it will get updated occasionally as I go along.
At the time, I had it pass word protected as a benefit to those who supported me. But that’s long gone now, and two events are coming that have convinced me it’s time to make it available to anyone who wants to have a peek at what I was doing.
First, the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS) is holding a webinar on science crowdfunding:
There is also going to be a symposium on social media at the next AAAS meeting in Boston in mid-February. Jai Ranganathan will be talking about #SciFund.
The second event this week that made me open up the expedition blog is Science Online 2013. Since that conference is all about things like #SciFund, I want to give fellow attendees a chance to see that part of my project.
External links
Amazons and Goliaths
I called it “Amazons and Goliaths” after the titles of my two #SciFund projects: Doctor Zen and the Amazon Crayfish Civilization and Beach of the Goliath Crabs. To read it in chronological order (recommended), start at the bottom of this page and read backwards. Pretend you’re reading manga.
The blog has been mostly quiet since November, but I’m starting to do some of the analysis of the data I got, and it will get updated occasionally as I go along.
At the time, I had it pass word protected as a benefit to those who supported me. But that’s long gone now, and two events are coming that have convinced me it’s time to make it available to anyone who wants to have a peek at what I was doing.
First, the American Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS) is holding a webinar on science crowdfunding:
Crowdfunding Science: Appealing to the online community for research money
Event Date: January 29, 2013 12 p.m. Eastern, 9 a.m. Pacific, 5 p.m. GMT, 6 p.m. CEST
With federal and state funding for science on the downward trend, many young scientists are bypassing the grant writing process and appealing directly to the public via the Internet for money to support their research. Crowdfunding, as it is known, holds huge potential for scientists who can effectively capture the imagination of the public and get them to open their wallets in support of science.
In AAAS MemberCentral’s webinar "Appealing to the online community for research money", we’ll look at #SciFund Challange, a website that helps researchers get their projects funded by the public, and we’ll also hear from two scientists who successfully funded their projects via the crowd. We’ll find out what they learned along the way, share tips on how to reach your funding goal and give you an opportunity to ask the panelists questions.
There is also going to be a symposium on social media at the next AAAS meeting in Boston in mid-February. Jai Ranganathan will be talking about #SciFund.
The second event this week that made me open up the expedition blog is Science Online 2013. Since that conference is all about things like #SciFund, I want to give fellow attendees a chance to see that part of my project.
External links
Amazons and Goliaths
25 January 2013
24 January 2013
Positive change
This article is one of the first since the new semester to describe the state of the plan to create a new university in South Texas.
Normally, I’d be on the bus to go to Austin to talk to the legislators, but this time, I’m already committed to flying to Science Online that day.
While I have my share of frustrations with my institution, I have to say this. It is nice to see a place that is moving in a generally positive way.
At a time when many institutions are ratcheting up the numbers of adjunct professors and reducing the number of tenure track faculty, our institution has gone in the exact opposite direction over the last ten to fifteen years. The number of adjuncts teaching classes has gone down.
In my department, when I joined, there was one woman on tenure track in the entire department; none were tenured. We’re still a long way from parity. I think I estimated we’d have as many men as women in the department when I was 85 or something. And several faculty members, both men and women, have started new families since taking the job here. Not saying it’s been easy or perfect for them, but it has to set a much better example for our students.
The big “if” for the new university and its med school is whether the Legislature approves the plan by a two-thirds vote. The usual UTPA day at the Capitol in Austin was renamed this year as RGV Higher Education Day at the Capitol. The UTPA delegation will visit legislators’ offices Jan. 30 and talk up the new university.
Normally, I’d be on the bus to go to Austin to talk to the legislators, but this time, I’m already committed to flying to Science Online that day.
Nelsen said the new university’s lack of a price tag for the state might be the biggest plus for the Legislature.
“I haven’t had anybody that I talked to say ‘no’,” he said. “Part of that is that there is no fiscal note. Everyone is saying ‘Well, money.’ But to put us together doesn’t cost anything.”
On the other hand, anything could happen.
“I have run into some skepticism,” Nelsen said. “Some say, ‘Do you really think you’ll get a two-thirds vote in any legislature, as divided as legislatures are nowadays?’”
While I have my share of frustrations with my institution, I have to say this. It is nice to see a place that is moving in a generally positive way.
At a time when many institutions are ratcheting up the numbers of adjunct professors and reducing the number of tenure track faculty, our institution has gone in the exact opposite direction over the last ten to fifteen years. The number of adjuncts teaching classes has gone down.
In my department, when I joined, there was one woman on tenure track in the entire department; none were tenured. We’re still a long way from parity. I think I estimated we’d have as many men as women in the department when I was 85 or something. And several faculty members, both men and women, have started new families since taking the job here. Not saying it’s been easy or perfect for them, but it has to set a much better example for our students.
Science Online 2013: “Blogging for the long haul”
Here is the next appetizer for the second of two Science Online 2013 sessions I’m co-moderating. “Blogging for the Long Haul” will be moderated by Scicurious and myself.
Note! This session will be streamed online to Science Online Watch Parties. Mark your calendars!
When I suggested this panel, I was thinking about strategies to deal with changing online ecosystems. Science blogging has stood the test of time. It’s lasted a good, solid, decade, and has gone from strength to strength. Yet we’ve also seen networks and platforms emerge, dissolve, and morph multiple times, with blogger packing up house and moving over and over.
There are issues like long term preservation of blogs, discussed here:
Certain science stories that are likely to be of great interest to science historians played themselves out mostly on the blogosphere (e.g., arsenic life). If blogs are lost, so much context for understanding that episode in science will also be lost. (Blog preservation will be discussed at another panel. Nominate science blogs for archiving here.)
Dynamic Ecology touches on issues closer to the heart of this panel in this post:
For other examples of the changing ecosystem, Tumblr carved out a niche for hyper-focused “single serve” blogs. Twitter has also significantly changed the blogosphere: lots of things that people used to blog now get tweeted instead. People respond to posts with tweets instead of commenting on the blogs.
Speaking of Twitter, Miriam Goldstein tweeted this:
Andrew Thaler replied:
And that’s only after five years. Imagine how you might feel at ten. That’s becoming a bigger part of the session than I originally expected: how to deal with fatigue, or writer’s block, or feeling unnoticed, or running headlong into the dip.
There are reasons a-plenty why you might feel burned out. When you first start out, there is nowhere to go but up. You see the number of hits accumulate, you see more people retweet or +1 your newest posts, and you have all these ideas that you have never talked about online that you want to share.
But eventually, your readership plateaus. You run into “second album slump,” as musicians know it. The first album contained the band’s ten best songs, tried and tested and honed by years playing them in bars. You can’t draw upon that tested back catalogue again for the second album.
27 and a Ph.D. epitomises the feeling that it’s all been done before.
Then there’s the green-eyed monster.
You might be blogging your little heart out. Then, someone who hasn’t been blogging as long as you is suddenly getting a lot of attention, has posts that keep getting retweeted and showing up on Ed Yong’s “I’ve got your missing links right here” weekly compilations, and joins some swanky science blogging network. Their post goes viral, while yours, on the same subject... doesn’t.
The Internet, she is capracious mistress.
What can you do to keep a blog going after the “new car smell” has faded? Obviously, we want to spend a good chunk of our session talking about this, it’s would be cruel to leave you hanging until then. Sci and I talked about a few tips that we have both used to keep us going.
1. Blog for yourself first. If you are being too driven by external indications of success (hits, visitors, etc.), you are likely to become discouraged, maybe too early. Seth Godin goes so far as to say that when it comes to a blog, “It doesn’t matter if anyone reads it.”
2. Schedule. Both Sci and I are great believers in the power of making a schedule and sticking to it. One post a week, once a month, it doesn’t matter, but it helps when you commit to a timetable. Neither of us are “I only write when I’m inspired” types.
3. Don’t self censor. “This is too old, too small, too flawed for a blog post.” But you can never tell what will resonate with people. Even two or three paragraphs looks substantial compared to a tweet, or a +1.
We will cover all that and much more! To end this post on a fun note, Chad Orzel does some filking with “Still Stuck on Paragraph Two.” If you can’t recall the tune, it’s here.
Update, 3 February 2013: A Storify of the session is available here.
Update, 4 February 2013: Scicurious has a wrap-up here that includes her Storify at the top, but if you scroll all the way down to the end, she has some more comments and thoughts.
Update, 5 February 2013: Char Orzel reflects on the session.
Green-eyed monster by Friday Felts on Flickr; sued under a Creative Commons license.
Note! This session will be streamed online to Science Online Watch Parties. Mark your calendars!
When I suggested this panel, I was thinking about strategies to deal with changing online ecosystems. Science blogging has stood the test of time. It’s lasted a good, solid, decade, and has gone from strength to strength. Yet we’ve also seen networks and platforms emerge, dissolve, and morph multiple times, with blogger packing up house and moving over and over.
There are issues like long term preservation of blogs, discussed here:
With the emergence of practices like open notebook science, science blogging, and science discussion forums a considerable amount of this content is being produced and presented on the web. If we do not act to collect this contemporary material, we may end up with more complete records of scientists’ unpublished notes and personal communication from previous eras than we do from our own.
Certain science stories that are likely to be of great interest to science historians played themselves out mostly on the blogosphere (e.g., arsenic life). If blogs are lost, so much context for understanding that episode in science will also be lost. (Blog preservation will be discussed at another panel. Nominate science blogs for archiving here.)
Dynamic Ecology touches on issues closer to the heart of this panel in this post:
Indeed, many established ecology blogs seem to be slowing down, especially over the last few months...
(I)f it is a trend, I hope it reverses itself. It’s hard to see blogging becoming a key way in which ecologists communicate ideas with one another if existing blogs (especially established ones) wind down. And since there’s a stronger incentive to blog once a “culture of blogging” exists, but not before, a field can’t really develop a culture of blogging in the first place unless there are some “early adopters”, pioneers like the folks who write the blogs listed above. If the early adopters themselves give it up before enough other folks have followed their lead, it’s hard to see how you ever get a critical mass.
For other examples of the changing ecosystem, Tumblr carved out a niche for hyper-focused “single serve” blogs. Twitter has also significantly changed the blogosphere: lots of things that people used to blog now get tweeted instead. People respond to posts with tweets instead of commenting on the blogs.
Speaking of Twitter, Miriam Goldstein tweeted this:
Was sad that your “Twilight of the Science Bloggers” session didn't make it. I definitely feel faded & dim.
Andrew Thaler replied:
I feel like I've been running on fumes for the last year. Would have been nice to talk about issues other than “yay-blogging”
And that’s only after five years. Imagine how you might feel at ten. That’s becoming a bigger part of the session than I originally expected: how to deal with fatigue, or writer’s block, or feeling unnoticed, or running headlong into the dip.
There are reasons a-plenty why you might feel burned out. When you first start out, there is nowhere to go but up. You see the number of hits accumulate, you see more people retweet or +1 your newest posts, and you have all these ideas that you have never talked about online that you want to share.
But eventually, your readership plateaus. You run into “second album slump,” as musicians know it. The first album contained the band’s ten best songs, tried and tested and honed by years playing them in bars. You can’t draw upon that tested back catalogue again for the second album.
27 and a Ph.D. epitomises the feeling that it’s all been done before.
At times I’ve gotten so discouraged by the internet overload that I’ve felt like everything I have to say has been said, and that maybe I should hang the blogging gloves once and for all.
Then there’s the green-eyed monster.
You might be blogging your little heart out. Then, someone who hasn’t been blogging as long as you is suddenly getting a lot of attention, has posts that keep getting retweeted and showing up on Ed Yong’s “I’ve got your missing links right here” weekly compilations, and joins some swanky science blogging network. Their post goes viral, while yours, on the same subject... doesn’t.
The Internet, she is capracious mistress.
What can you do to keep a blog going after the “new car smell” has faded? Obviously, we want to spend a good chunk of our session talking about this, it’s would be cruel to leave you hanging until then. Sci and I talked about a few tips that we have both used to keep us going.
1. Blog for yourself first. If you are being too driven by external indications of success (hits, visitors, etc.), you are likely to become discouraged, maybe too early. Seth Godin goes so far as to say that when it comes to a blog, “It doesn’t matter if anyone reads it.”
2. Schedule. Both Sci and I are great believers in the power of making a schedule and sticking to it. One post a week, once a month, it doesn’t matter, but it helps when you commit to a timetable. Neither of us are “I only write when I’m inspired” types.
3. Don’t self censor. “This is too old, too small, too flawed for a blog post.” But you can never tell what will resonate with people. Even two or three paragraphs looks substantial compared to a tweet, or a +1.
We will cover all that and much more! To end this post on a fun note, Chad Orzel does some filking with “Still Stuck on Paragraph Two.” If you can’t recall the tune, it’s here.
Update, 3 February 2013: A Storify of the session is available here.
Update, 4 February 2013: Scicurious has a wrap-up here that includes her Storify at the top, but if you scroll all the way down to the end, she has some more comments and thoughts.
Update, 5 February 2013: Char Orzel reflects on the session.
Green-eyed monster by Friday Felts on Flickr; sued under a Creative Commons license.
23 January 2013
Science Online 2013: “Open access or vanity press” appetizer
Recently, I got an email that began:
Are you for real? Reviewer or Editor-in-Chief. You know, whatever. It’s cool.
For working scientists, this is something that rings alarm bells. It’s simply not the way business is done at other journals. But is that necessarily bad? This turns out to be a particularly timely example of issues related to one of the sessions I’ll be moderating at Science Online 2013 next week. Chris Gunter and I will be co-moderating the session, “Open access or vanity press?” Here is a warm-up, an opening gambit, an appetizer...
The OA Interviews: Ahmed Hindawi, founder of Hindawi Publishing Corporation - Open and Shut? (17 September 2012)
If you read only one thing in preparation for the session, I suggest the introduction to this interview. It’s 20 pages long (not the introduction and interview, the introduction alone), but it is comprehensive and substantial interview that deserves the “#longreads” hashtag. But that introduction is one of the best descriptions of the issues around the problems of legitimacy of open access publishing that you will find.
If you’re looking for a shorter synopsis, perhaps this one pager will work for you.
As Open Access Explodes, How to Tell The Good From the Bad and the Ugly? - Enserink M. 2012. Science 338(6110): 1018. DOI: 10.1126/science.338.6110.1018
If you want to move from theory to practice, and how this plagues working researchers, especially people early in their careers, check this two part story at Nothing in Biology Makes Sense! It encapsulates the problem in a nutshell:
Predatory Open-Access Journals? - Nothing in Biology Makes Sense! (12 July 2012)
Predatory Open-Access Journals: Part 2 - Nothing in Biology Makes Sense! (14 July 2012)
That pair of posts sort of sums up the conundrum. Going back some time, we find some ways that people have assessed quality by other means.
Adventure in open access publishing - The Scholarly Kitchen (12 March 2009)
While this experiment did not result in acceptance, a second, similar experiment did.
Open Access Publisher Accepts Nonsense Manuscript for Dollars - The Scholarly Kitchen (10 July 2009)
This is a key story that was widely reported and contributed to the poor reputation of open access journals.
The publisher that fell victim to the first hoax in 2009, Bentham, has also received other poor press for perpetually bugging people.
Really sick of Bentham Open Spam - Phylogenomics (30 June 2009)
For $&%# sake, Bentham Open Journals, leave me alone - Phylogenomics (19 November 2009)
The rise of science spam - Neuroskeptic (27 November 2012)
This raises the question, though, of how a new journal can ever gain legitimacy. Even PLOS ONE, a success by most standards you’d care to name, was new once.
The Bentham case in 2009 was not the only example of a hoax paper accepted for publication. Another occurred last fall.
Mathgen paper accepted! - That’s mathematics! (14 September 2012)
Math Journal Accepts Nonsense Paper Generated by Computer Program - Geekosystem (19 October 2012)
Analyzing the math paper hoax, Bob O’Hara thinks molehills are made into mountains.
Open Access: credit where it’s due - GrrlScientist (26 October 2012)
Jeffrey Beall, who is mentioned above, is one of the names that comes up most frequently in discussions of predatory open access journals. He maintains a list of suspect publishers, and investigates new publishers. Here’s an example of his investigative work:
Copying Elsevier - Scholarly Open Access, 16 October 2012
Beall is often called upon to comment upon open access publishing.
Predatory publishers are corrupting open access by Jeffrey Beall, Nature (13 September 2012)
On the problem of “predatory open-access publishers” - SV-POW! (17 September 2012)
Crowdsouring a database of “predatory OA journals” - SV-POW!, 7 December 2012
Indeed, Beall’s influence is large enough that he was targeted by a smear campaign that claimed he would consider removing publishers from his list if he was paid $5,000. I grabbed the text from a forum around 10:00 am one morning, but it was gone by 10:30 am or so:
This was untrue, Beall said.
Beall’s solution to the problem of open access versus credibility?
On Predatory Publishers: a Q&A With Jeffrey Beall - Chronicle of Higher Education (5 June 2012)
One of the aspects that I did not initially consider in proposing this topic was the cultural issues involved. Developing, non-“Western” nations seem to be not only the source of, but fall prey to, much of the bogus publishing.
“Suspect” journals take scientists for a ride - SciDevNet, 21 January 2013
Coincidentally, Nature reported on this relationship just the next day:
Price doesn't always buy prestige in open access - Nature, 22 January 2013
Meanwhile, the biggest and most successful open access journal, PLOS ONE, continues to be a lightning rod. Here are just a few tweets:
“Many people have flat out said PLOS ONE papers won't count” (For tenure / promotion - ZF) – Lewis Lab
“I want to be an ally. But PLOS ONE isn't helping. (People) less attuned equate (Open Access) with low bar. Not good for science.” – E.G. Moss
“I’ve now heard from several tenured or near-tenure profs that publishing in @plosone was career suicide. Thanks a lot Open Access” – Ethan Perlstein
“A search (committee) chair once told me ‘PLOS ONE papers don't count, they publish anything’” – Eric J. Deeds
I have my own posts on this matter.
As Nigeria is to banking, India is to science publishing - NeuroDojo (6 January 2010)
More journals that smell like spam - NeuroDojo (29 January 2010)
Post-publication peer review - NeuroDojo (20 September 2010)
Additional, 24 January 2013:
Advice: how to decide where to submit your paper – Dynamic Ecology (24 January 2013)
I’m getting increasingly interested in the line between “not viewed highly” and “predatory.” If two publishers take money for article fees, and your article is published, but neither is viewed by colleagues as “counting,” arguing over the relative rigour of peer review is almost moot.
Update, 8 February 2013: Here is a Storify of the session, courtesy of Doctor Free-Ride:
“My Life in Tweets” photo by STML on Flickr; raptor picture by Andrea Westmoreland on Flickr; both used under a Creative Commons license.
You are invited as a reviewer/a member of editorial board/an editor-in-chief of [Journal name].
Are you for real? Reviewer or Editor-in-Chief. You know, whatever. It’s cool.
For working scientists, this is something that rings alarm bells. It’s simply not the way business is done at other journals. But is that necessarily bad? This turns out to be a particularly timely example of issues related to one of the sessions I’ll be moderating at Science Online 2013 next week. Chris Gunter and I will be co-moderating the session, “Open access or vanity press?” Here is a warm-up, an opening gambit, an appetizer...
The OA Interviews: Ahmed Hindawi, founder of Hindawi Publishing Corporation - Open and Shut? (17 September 2012)
The speed of Hindawi’s growth, which included creating many new journals in a short space of time and mass mailing researchers, led to suspicion that it was a “predatory” organisation. Today, however, most of its detractors have been won round and — bar the occasional hiccup — Hindawi is viewed as a respectable and responsible publisher.
Nevertheless, Hindawi’s story poses a number of questions. First, how do researchers distinguish between good and bad publishers in today’s Internet-fuelled publishing revolution, and what constitutes acceptable practice anyway?
If you read only one thing in preparation for the session, I suggest the introduction to this interview. It’s 20 pages long (not the introduction and interview, the introduction alone), but it is comprehensive and substantial interview that deserves the “#longreads” hashtag. But that introduction is one of the best descriptions of the issues around the problems of legitimacy of open access publishing that you will find.
If you’re looking for a shorter synopsis, perhaps this one pager will work for you.
As Open Access Explodes, How to Tell The Good From the Bad and the Ugly? - Enserink M. 2012. Science 338(6110): 1018. DOI: 10.1126/science.338.6110.1018
Picking the right journal has always been difficult, but the plethora of new open access (OA) journals — and the increasing pressure from funders to publish in them — has made the choice even more daunting.
If you want to move from theory to practice, and how this plagues working researchers, especially people early in their careers, check this two part story at Nothing in Biology Makes Sense! It encapsulates the problem in a nutshell:
Predatory Open-Access Journals? - Nothing in Biology Makes Sense! (12 July 2012)
So I submitted to this journal, after looking up some of their papers and a few people that have published there and convincing myself it wasn’t a flat-out scam.
One day after I submitted, I got an email asking me to review my own article. I know, right? How could that ever happen with a legitimate journal? ...
Then, this morning, I got final acceptance of the manuscript and I’m not sure what to do.
Predatory Open-Access Journals: Part 2 - Nothing in Biology Makes Sense! (14 July 2012)
There were many good reasons, in my mind, to just do it – it was peer-reviewed, my article and software are sound (though a minor contribution) and I would like closure on this project I finished a year ago. It turns out, there are some even better reasons not to publish with this journal.
That pair of posts sort of sums up the conundrum. Going back some time, we find some ways that people have assessed quality by other means.
Adventure in open access publishing - The Scholarly Kitchen (12 March 2009)
Just how desperate was this publisher for a manuscript? Would they accept just any submission as long as I was willing to pay their $800 publication fee? I decided to embark on a little experiment.
While this experiment did not result in acceptance, a second, similar experiment did.
Open Access Publisher Accepts Nonsense Manuscript for Dollars - The Scholarly Kitchen (10 July 2009)
This is a key story that was widely reported and contributed to the poor reputation of open access journals.
The publisher that fell victim to the first hoax in 2009, Bentham, has also received other poor press for perpetually bugging people.
Really sick of Bentham Open Spam - Phylogenomics (30 June 2009)
This is basically a form of SPAM as they send these out to people no matter what the connection is to the journals field.
For $&%# sake, Bentham Open Journals, leave me alone - Phylogenomics (19 November 2009)
The most annoying part to me of Bentham Open is that they try to make it seem that anything published in an Open Access journal is better than anything published in a non Open Access journal. While I personally believe publishing in an OA manner is great, lying about the benefits of OA is not a good thing.
The rise of science spam - Neuroskeptic (27 November 2012)
If you are resorting to spam to get to people to write for your journal, I don’t ever want to read it and will never cite anything published in it.
Even if you were only angling for readers, I’d be suspicious of your integrity, but to spam for people to submit to you is absurd. Even mediocre journals nowadays get far more submissions than they can ever print. So if your journal isn’t even mediocre enough to attract people then you have a real problem.
This raises the question, though, of how a new journal can ever gain legitimacy. Even PLOS ONE, a success by most standards you’d care to name, was new once.
The Bentham case in 2009 was not the only example of a hoax paper accepted for publication. Another occurred last fall.
Mathgen paper accepted! - That’s mathematics! (14 September 2012)
I’m pleased to announce that Mathgen has had its first randomly-generated paper accepted by a reputable journal!
Math Journal Accepts Nonsense Paper Generated by Computer Program - Geekosystem (19 October 2012)
(I)t’s randomly generated nonsense – grammatically accurate sentences penned by a computer program that have no mathematical merit, so seriously, don’t feel bad if it doesn’t make sense to you. You know who should feel bad, though? The person at the open access math journal Advances in Pure Mathematics who accepted this paper for publication.
Analyzing the math paper hoax, Bob O’Hara thinks molehills are made into mountains.
Open Access: credit where it’s due - GrrlScientist (26 October 2012)
The journal that accepted the randomly-generated paper is published by SCIRP, and is on Jeffrey Beall’s List of Predatory, Open-Access Publishers. In other words, we know from their behaviour that they essentially act as vanity publishers for scientists. Based on the number of spam emails I receive from them, I suspect they’ve managed to become quite well known for this. Which means that nobody will think highly of a paper published in one of their journals, so very few scientists will want to submit a paper to them: you simply don't get any credit from your peers for publishing there &ndash indeed, they may even laugh at you behind your back.
Jeffrey Beall, who is mentioned above, is one of the names that comes up most frequently in discussions of predatory open access journals. He maintains a list of suspect publishers, and investigates new publishers. Here’s an example of his investigative work:
Copying Elsevier - Scholarly Open Access, 16 October 2012
The journal gratuitously uses the Elsevier logo in a prominent position on its homepage. It also lists an impact factor — which it does not have.
To compete in a crowded market, predatory journals need to look as prestigious and authentic as possible. The publishers of this journal have done a spiffy job of creating a website that makes it looks like their journal is an Elsevier journal. ...
It didn’t take me very long to find plagiarism in the journal’s articles.
Beall is often called upon to comment upon open access publishing.
Predatory publishers are corrupting open access by Jeffrey Beall, Nature (13 September 2012)
Honest scientists stand to lose the most in this unethical quagmire. When a researcher’s work is published alongside articles that are plagiarized, that report on conclusions gained from unsound methodologies or that contain altered photographic figures, it becomes tainted by association. Unethical scientists gaming the system are earning tenure and promotion at the expense of the honest.
On the problem of “predatory open-access publishers” - SV-POW! (17 September 2012)
(T)his issue is nothing to do with OA: there always have been and always will be fraudulent journals and publishers alongside the good ones; and it always has been and always will be authors’ responsibility to avoid them and go to the good places instead.
Crowdsouring a database of “predatory OA journals” - SV-POW!, 7 December 2012
My feeling is that, while a good solution could certainly say positive things about good publishers as well as negative things about bad publishers, we do need it to produce (among other things) a blacklist, if only to be an alternative to Beall’s one. Since that’s the only game in town, it has altogether too much power at the moment.
Indeed, Beall’s influence is large enough that he was targeted by a smear campaign that claimed he would consider removing publishers from his list if he was paid $5,000. I grabbed the text from a forum around 10:00 am one morning, but it was gone by 10:30 am or so:
I was surprised when one of our editors told me that the name of Ashdin Publishing is found in the list of "Beall's List: Potential, possible, or probable predatory scholarly open-access publishers" ... After I received the e-mail below, I am not any more surprised. Now, I am sure that the author, irrespective the good reasons he may has for preparing this list, wants to blackmail small publishers to pay him.
-------- Original Message --------
Subject: Open Access Publishing
Date: Mon, 03 Dec 2012 17:39:18 +0000
From: Jeffrey Beall
To: info@ashdin.com
I maintain list of predatory open access publishers in my blog http://scholarlyoa.com
Your publisher name is also included in 2012 edition of my predatory open access publishers list. My recent article in Nature journal can be read below
http://www.nature.co...-access-1.11385
I can consider re-evaluating your journals for 2013 edition of my list. It takes a lot my time and resources. The fee for re-evaluation of your publisher is USD 5000. If your publisher name is not in my list, it will increase trustworthiness to your journals and it will draw more article submissions. In case you like re-evaluation for your journals, you can contact me.
Cordially
Jeffrey Beall
This was untrue, Beall said.
I've been a victim of email spoofing in which someone is sending emails that appear to be from me but really are not.
One of the spoofed emails is an offer to "reevaluate" a publisher's presence on my list for five thousand dollars. These emails try to make it look like I am extorting money from publishers.
Beall’s solution to the problem of open access versus credibility?
On Predatory Publishers: a Q&A With Jeffrey Beall - Chronicle of Higher Education (5 June 2012)
I support what I call “platinum open-access.” This is open-access without author fees, and with the publication costs supported by volunteer work and benevolent funders. There are a few publishers that now use this model, but it’s not sustainable and it’s not scalable to all of scholarly communication. The only truly successful model that I have seen is the traditional publishing model.
One of the aspects that I did not initially consider in proposing this topic was the cultural issues involved. Developing, non-“Western” nations seem to be not only the source of, but fall prey to, much of the bogus publishing.
“Suspect” journals take scientists for a ride - SciDevNet, 21 January 2013
Young researchers in developing countries can be easy prey. The pressure to publish has risen dramatically because career advancement depends on it — publications can embellish a job application. ...
Researchers often do not realise they are being duped, he says. “In fact, the assumption is entrenched in academic circles in Nigeria that the higher the fee charged [by the journal], the higher the quality.”
Coincidentally, Nature reported on this relationship just the next day:
Price doesn't always buy prestige in open access - Nature, 22 January 2013
The “real goal”, West says, is to help to create a transparent market in open-access publishing. “We hope to clean up a little of the predatory publishing, where publishers might be charging more than their value merits.”
The tool, called Cost Effectiveness for Open Access Journals, incorporates pricing and prestige information for 657 open-access journals indexed by Thomson Reuters, including 356 that do not charge any fees.
Meanwhile, the biggest and most successful open access journal, PLOS ONE, continues to be a lightning rod. Here are just a few tweets:
“Many people have flat out said PLOS ONE papers won't count” (For tenure / promotion - ZF) – Lewis Lab
“I want to be an ally. But PLOS ONE isn't helping. (People) less attuned equate (Open Access) with low bar. Not good for science.” – E.G. Moss
“I’ve now heard from several tenured or near-tenure profs that publishing in @plosone was career suicide. Thanks a lot Open Access” – Ethan Perlstein
“A search (committee) chair once told me ‘PLOS ONE papers don't count, they publish anything’” – Eric J. Deeds
I have my own posts on this matter.
As Nigeria is to banking, India is to science publishing - NeuroDojo (6 January 2010)
I recently received this in my inbox. And I was all, like, “Whoa.” It was like I’d traveled back in time to the early 1990s and landed on an old GeoCities page. The explosion of typefaces and random colours, the spelling mistakes, the random religious element... slap on a page counter and some blinking text, and it would be indistinguishable.
But no, this is supposed to be from a serious scientific journal. Excuse me, “Joournal”.
More journals that smell like spam - NeuroDojo (29 January 2010)
It’s be great that the publishing revolution creates the potential for new journals. But from a research author’s point of view, it’s the wild, wild west out there. And it’s not clear yet who are the honest settlers and who are the cattle barons and con men.
Post-publication peer review - NeuroDojo (20 September 2010)
It’s incredibly tempting to send in a hoax article to see if their “post-publication peer review” calls it out as gibberish.I look forward to hearing the thoughts of those at Science Online next week!
Additional, 24 January 2013:
Advice: how to decide where to submit your paper – Dynamic Ecology (24 January 2013)
There are those who feel strongly that selecting papers on the basis of “interest”, “novelty”, “importance”, and other such attributes is a purely arbitrary business with no place in science. I don’t take that view. But if you feel that way, you need to decide whether you’re prepared to live by your principles and submit all your work to PLOS ONE or other unselective journals, given that many of your colleagues do not share your views and may not view your CV very highly.
I’m getting increasingly interested in the line between “not viewed highly” and “predatory.” If two publishers take money for article fees, and your article is published, but neither is viewed by colleagues as “counting,” arguing over the relative rigour of peer review is almost moot.
Update, 8 February 2013: Here is a Storify of the session, courtesy of Doctor Free-Ride:
“My Life in Tweets” photo by STML on Flickr; raptor picture by Andrea Westmoreland on Flickr; both used under a Creative Commons license.
22 January 2013
The Revisionaries to air nationally in U.S.
One of the things that got me “born again hard” about blogging was following the development of the Texas K-12 science standards, mostly back in around 2009. One of the unexpected outcomes of that process was that a documentary film got made. Originally titled Standing up to the Experts, it was partially funded by a Kickstarter campaign. I was one of the backers.
The final film, retitled The Revisionaries, will start showing next week across the U.S. on public television stations, according to the National Center for Science Education. On my local station, it will be airing three times, but two of those are in the dead of night.
I’m looking forward to seeing the result at long last.
Additional, 25 January 2013: The Panda’s Thumb is reporting that PBS is screening a substantially shortened version of the movie. Their post also has a mini-review:
McLeroy could be an excellent, even inspiring teacher, if only he were not so badly misinformed.
Additional, 28 January 2012: The creators have an “Ask Me Anything” thread on Reddit.
Related posts
“The war on science is officially back on in Texas”
Standing Up to the Experts documentary funded
Standing Up to the Experts becomes The Revisionaries
Reviews for The Revisionaries movie
Tuesday Crustie: Bubbles!
Love the colours in this crab!
Photo by Leonard John Matthews on Flickr; used under a Creative Commons license.
21 January 2013
The pros and cons of doing a master’s degree
I am the program coordinator for the biology master’s degree at my university, so I obviously have a vested interest in this question. A conversation about it broke out on Twitter on Sunday, which I present here without commentary (yet).
18 January 2013
Use easy words
xkcd did a brilliant comic where he explained one of the most magnificent machines ever built by humans, the Saturn V rocket, using only the most common words in English.Of course, “rocket” is a rare word in English, so he rechristened it “up goer.”
Now, Theo Sanderson has done the scientific community a great service by creating a little editor that lets you see if you can write something using only the most common English words. And, of course, scientists are rising to the challenge, trying to describe their research in words anyone should know. Someone has started a Tumblr of scientists explaining their research this way.
It’s so good that I’ll forgive that it uses American English. (“Colour” is not allowed, but “color” is.)
Given that just yesterday I was talking about my science not being sophisticated, I didn’t find it a hard task:
I am so using this in my next Biological Writing class.
P.S.—My bit on the Tumblr!
Additional, 19 January 2013: There’s another Tumblr, with this one devoted specifically to telling Ph.D. theses in the “UpGoer Five” style. I did my dissertation abstract here, because I though the first one I did wasa little too easy, as it was so general. I wanted to see how I did with a specific piece of technical writing.
It takes a while, but I have to say, I don’t find it very hard to convert most of my work. And you can see the benefit of a little jargon: you can say more in fewer words.
Now, Theo Sanderson has done the scientific community a great service by creating a little editor that lets you see if you can write something using only the most common English words. And, of course, scientists are rising to the challenge, trying to describe their research in words anyone should know. Someone has started a Tumblr of scientists explaining their research this way.
It’s so good that I’ll forgive that it uses American English. (“Colour” is not allowed, but “color” is.)
Given that just yesterday I was talking about my science not being sophisticated, I didn’t find it a hard task:
I work with ten legged animals that are hard on the outside and that live in the water. I watch what they do, and sometimes I look at their brains to see how they do it.
Some of my animals are all girls, and don't need boys to make babies. They are interesting, and many people like to keep them in their homes, but they could be a problem if they are let go in the wrong place.
Some of my animals live in very very small rocks along the edge of the water, and go through it almost like it wasn't there. I check out where they live, how big they are, and what colour they are.
I am so using this in my next Biological Writing class.
P.S.—My bit on the Tumblr!
Additional, 19 January 2013: There’s another Tumblr, with this one devoted specifically to telling Ph.D. theses in the “UpGoer Five” style. I did my dissertation abstract here, because I though the first one I did wasa little too easy, as it was so general. I wanted to see how I did with a specific piece of technical writing.
Some animals that are hard on the outside and have ten legs have come up with a “new” means of getting around: they use their legs to go through very small rocks instead of walking around on the bottom like other ten legged animals. I looked at how three of these animals from two families get around. There are several things in how these three animals move that are almost the same in all three, suggesting that they know how to go through small rocks because they all got it from older animals in their family tree. The way their legs go around is much the same in all three animals: when you look at an animal from its side, leg 4 goes around one direction, and legs 2 and 3 go around in the other direction. In all three animals, the left and right legs take turns going forwards and back; the back end of the animal goes forward and back faster than the legs; and the timing of moving in a one given leg is much the same in two of the three animals. There are also things that make animals in the two families different. When two of the animals get around, the left and right legs take turns at the start, but then change to moving forward and back at the same time. In the third kind of animal, the left and right legs 2 and 3 take turns all the time, but the left and right legs 4 can still move together, and move faster than legs 2 and 3 (about as fast as the back end of the animal). There are also some things that almost but not quite the same in how these animals get around and how other animals walk, suggesting these two ways of getting around may have started the same way. The way the legs on one side of an animal move is almost the same whether an animal is moving through small rocks or walking, and the cells that control the end of the legs are almost the same in animals that move through small rocks and those that walk. Moving through small rocks and walking look different: the back end of the animals moves fast when it is going through small rocks but not walking, and that legs of animals moving through small rocks move in very few ways, while the legs of walking animals can move in many ways. The way legs 2 and 3 move when going through small rocks looks like the way walking legs move when an animal is walking back, but the way leg 4 moves when going through small rocks looks more like how walking legs move when an animal is walking forward. This suggests that moving through small rocks came from joining several different ways that animals move around (walking back in legs 2 and 3, forward walking in leg 4, and flipping the back end very fast).
It takes a while, but I have to say, I don’t find it very hard to convert most of my work. And you can see the benefit of a little jargon: you can say more in fewer words.
What we know and don’t know about crustacean pain
Summary
What we know about crustacean pain?
What don’t we know?
Paper unpacking
A new paper on crustacean pain by Magee and Elwood came out on Wednesday, 16 January 2013. This new paper is, in some ways, a variation on papers that senior author Robert Elwood published on hermit crabs (Appel and Elwood 2009, Elwood and Appel, 2009). In these earlier hermit crab work, they shocked the abdomen of the crabs, and they found they could get the hermits to leave the shell and take what would normally be an inferior shell as a new home.
In the new article, Magee and Elwood used electric shock as their stimulus again. Electric shock is used in many studies of pain, but it does have a problem: it’s not very specific. It will trip off any kind of cell that is electrically excitable, including sensory neurons, motor neurons (potentially causing spikes from motor neurons to travel backwards into the central nervous system), and muscles. It’s difficult to know just what you’re doing to the animal’s nervous system.
Crabs (in this case, Carcinus maenas; pictured) like dark hidey-holes. Magee and Elwood took the crabs, placed them in an open arena with two shelters, one with vertical stripes and one with horizontal stripes. Then, they chose one shelter that if the crab entered it, they gave it an electric shock. They gave the animals 10 trials, which were delivered in short order (two minutes between each trial). The total testing time for each animal seemed to be maybe an hour or two, depending on the crab’s behaviour.
What you would expect is that crabs would learn to avoid the avoid the shelter in which they got shocked. This is indeed what happens (Figure 1 from Magee and Elwood below). But notice that even at the end of the trails, about a third of the crabs are still walking into the shock box. Maybe these are just crab masochists.
The authors went on to test whether it was the stripes on the shelter, or the direction of the shelter, that the crabs had learned by swapping the positions of the two shelters. This seemed to have happened right after the ten trials. Given that part of the argument that they make in the Introduction that “pain facilitates long-term protection because of the ease with which animals learn to avoid that situation and avoid future damage,” and in the Discussion that they are finding “long-term motivational change,” the relatively short time scale is slightly surprising. Magee and Elwood’s argument would be greatly strengthened if they showed the crabs retained the memory after, say, 24 hours or 48 hours. Other studies have shown that decapod crustaceans, like crayfish and lobsters, can remember things, such as who they’ve fought, for days to weeks (reviewed in Hemsworth et al. 2007; see their Table 1).
It’s worth comparing this to an earlier paper that had a similar approach. Kawai et al. (2004) also applied electric shock to crayfish. Those authors had two groups:
These authors noted that group F, on the right in the figure below, learned to avoid the shock. But notice that about half the animals in this group never learned the task (S6, 7, and 10), and it took a long time. Group B never improved.
Magee and Elwood mention the long training in the paper by Kawai and colleagues, but not the variation in performance either between the two groups, or within the one group. I wonder if an animal by animal breakdown in the Magee and Elwood paper would show the same sort of individual variation. That is, the Magee and Elwood figure could either be:
Now, the crayfish were shocked less than the crabs were. But if the idea being put forward is that avoidance learning suggests pain, would this mean that only about half of crayfish feel pain... and only if they are looking at a door?
Press coverage
Having talked about the paper itself, I want to shift to how it’s being reported. I expected media attention over this paper, because previous papers from the Elwood lab had gotten attention. For instance, a lead interview on Quirks and Quarks. I was still surprised by the breadth of coverage people in my Twitter feed helped me find yesterday. The Daily Mail was the first I found, followed by the BBC, Discovery, The Guardian, National Public Radio, Fox News, Live Science and I’m sure there will be many others.
The Mail article starts off with the old hack, “Scientists have proven...” as though this is a worldwide consensus instead of two people (Magee and Elwood, 2013). Oh, but how such reports grate on me with their use of the word “prove.” (There are exceptions; the BBC puts “further evidence” in its headline, which emphasizes that this is part of a series of studies).
The original paper in JEB is, as often the case, much more nuanced than the press articles suggest. It carefully uses phrases like, “consistent with pain”, which admits there are alternative hypotheses. Elwood and colleagues are doing a series of studies, step by step; the sort of incremental progress that characterizes so much of science.
Let me start by saying that not everyone agrees with the notion generally that crustaceans feel pain. Victoria Braithwaite, in her book Do Fish Feel Pain? (reviewed here), concluded that crustaceans do not feel pain, even after long discussions with Elwood.
The two have very different standards on what they think the criteria for pain should be. Elwood appears to think that to show that an animal feels pain, you only need to show that it responds to a nasty stimulus with something more complicated than a reflex. Braithwaite thinks that to show an animal feels pain, you need to show that animal is capable of consciousness. I think it’s fair to say that neither one is widely accepted as the standard for showing an animal feels pain.
To take a similar case, the evidence for nociception in fish has been clear for a decade now: fish have neurons specialized for tissue damage. Braithwaite gave a book-length argument in Do Feel Feel Pain? why she was convinced fish feel pain. And yet, just weeks ago, a new review article by Rose and colleagues (in press) answered the rhetorical question in its title “Do fish really feel pain?” (my emphasis; I can’t see the title as anything other than a very deliberate snub of Braithwaite’s book) with a definite, “No.”
If people are still engaged in heated debates about pain in fish after ten years, even when the evidence related to the question is much further developed than it is for crustaceans, we are a long way from “proving” crustaceans feel pain.
Following along those lines of whether crustaceans need specific conditions to experience pain, I want to return to the issue of specific stimuli. Here is one of my issues with how this story is being reported:
Electric shock is not boiling water.
Many sources are immediately linking the two things under the common notion of “pain.” But only one was tested in the paper. The Daily Mail wrote:
So did Discovery:
The Guardian also tied this research to cooking:
So did Cosmos magazine (mirrored at ABC):
So did Fox News, and added housing into the mix.
Even if you agree that electric shock is painful to the crabs, that does not automatically mean that crabs experience pain the same way we do. It’s not a crazy hypothesis, but it is still something to be tested rather than something that has already been demonstrated. For instance, at the recent Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology meeting, one talk showed a short clip rather like this one.
It shows a crab, its claw injured in a fight, crabbing its claw, and ripping it off. You could hear the audience reacting when this was shown; people made the sort of groans that they make when they see the weekly sports blooper reel, particularly when you see accidental hits in the groin area.
Here’s a shorter, (but off-colour!) video of the same behaviour.
This is a reminder that what might seem painful to us is not necessarily painful to a different animal. Tearing off a limb voluntarily is not something a human would do.
Let me give you another quick example.
In humans, capsaicin is the chemical that makes chili peppers hot, and is often used in studies of pain as a painful stimuli. Strictly speaking, the chemical causes sensory neurons called nociceptors to fire. Fruit flies also have nociceptors , so at first blush, it’s perfectly reasonably to think they would avoid food laced with capsaicin.
Nope. The more capsaicin, the more the flies like the food (Al-Anzi et al. 2006). Live Science also earns points in my book for mentioning that naked mole rats do not respond to many stimuli that other mammals avoid and appear painful.
If you want to know if boiling a lobster is painful to said lobster, you should test its response to high temperatures, not electric shock.
It is heartening to see that some of the articles I’ve found obviously used embargo time to find other scientists to comment on the study. Scientists who study fish (Paul Hart, quoted in The Guardian; Lynne Sneddon, quoted by the BBC; Lynne worked on fish nociception, so was a good choice), marine organisms besides decapod crustaceans (Magnus Johnson, also quoted in The Guardian). Only one reported contacted someone who has worked on crustacean behaviour (Francesca Gherhardi, who I just met last week, quoted by Live Science).
Having provided several papers providing evidence that electric shock is painful, I am surprised that NPR reports that Elwood suggests electrocuting crustaceans is a pain-free way of dispatching them.
I’ve commented on this before (here, here). In crustacean neurobiology labs, the most commonly used anaesthetic for crabs, crayfish, and lobster, is chilling. My suggestion would be to put an animal on crushed ice.
I do not have strong opinions about whether crustaceans feel pain or not. But I do think that this new paper is closer to the beginning of research on this kind of question than it is to the end.
Additional: I find it interesting that The Independent ran this story in their “Food and drink” section.
References
Al-Anzi B, Tracey Jr. WD, Benzer S. 2006. Response of Drosophila to wasabi is mediated by painless, the fly homolog of mammalian TRPA1/ANKTM1. Current Biology 16(10): 1034-1040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.002
Appel M, Elwood RW. 2009. Motivational trade-offs and potential pain experience in hermit crabs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 119: 120-124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.03.013
Braithwaite VA. 2010. Do Fish Feel Pain? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Elwood RW, Appel M. 2009. Pain experience in hermit crabs? Animal Behaviour 77: 1243-1246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028
Hemsworth R, Villareal W, Patullo BW, Macmillan DL. 2007. Crustacean social behavioral changes in response to isolation. The Biological Bulletin 213(2): 187-195. http://www.biolbull.org/cgi/content/abstract/213/2/187
Kawai N, Kono R, Sugimoto S. 2004. Avoidance learning in the crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) depends on the predatory imminence of the unconditioned stimulus: a behavior systems approach to learning in invertebrates. Behavioural Brain Research 150(1-2): 229-237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00261-4
Magee B, Elwood RW. 2013. Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain. The Journal of Experimental Biology 216(3): 353-358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.072041
Rose JD, Arlinghaus R, Cooke SJ, Diggles BK, Sawynok W, Stevens ED, Wynne CDL. 2012. Can fish really feel pain? Fish and Fisheries: in press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/faf.12010
Related posts
Ignorance may not be bliss, but perhaps it is painless
Crustacean nociception: Origins, part 1
Crustacean nociception: Origins, part 2
Crustacean nociception: the worry
Shell shock
Shell shock, revisited
Tenth International Congress for Neuroethology, Day 5 (nociception symposium coverage)
External links
The Daily Mail: Lobsters and crabs can feel pain and take action to avoid it happening again
Live Science: Crabs really do feel pain: Study (Mirrored by Huffington Post: Crabs Feel Pain After All, New Study Suggests)
NPR The Salt: Yes, Virginia, Crabs Likely Feel Pain, But They're Still Delicious
Fox News: Lobsters, crabs feel pain, scientists say
BBC: Further evidence crabs and other crustaceans feel pain
The Guardian: Crab study puts pain on the menu
Cosmos magazine: Be kind to your seafood, study urges
Australian Broadcasting Corporation (ABC) Science: Be kind to your seafood, study urges
The Independent: Shock survey: Lobsters and crabs feel pain
Crab photo by BrentMWilson on Flickr; used under a Creative Commons license.
What we know about crustacean pain?
- Crabs, and probably other big decapod crustaceans, avoid electric shock in the short term.
- They can learn to avoid places where they were shocked over slightly longer terms.
- There may be substantial variation across individuals in their ability to learn.
- The evidence is consistent with pain.
- Pain is hard to prove, even in humans.
What don’t we know?
- Whether electric shock is normally relevant to crustaceans.
- Whether electric shock processed in the same sort of way that people and mammals process noxious stimuli.
- Whether there are specialized neurons for noxious stimuli in crustacean.
- Whether any other kinds of stimuli are aversive to large crustaceans, like high temperatures.
Paper unpacking
A new paper on crustacean pain by Magee and Elwood came out on Wednesday, 16 January 2013. This new paper is, in some ways, a variation on papers that senior author Robert Elwood published on hermit crabs (Appel and Elwood 2009, Elwood and Appel, 2009). In these earlier hermit crab work, they shocked the abdomen of the crabs, and they found they could get the hermits to leave the shell and take what would normally be an inferior shell as a new home.
In the new article, Magee and Elwood used electric shock as their stimulus again. Electric shock is used in many studies of pain, but it does have a problem: it’s not very specific. It will trip off any kind of cell that is electrically excitable, including sensory neurons, motor neurons (potentially causing spikes from motor neurons to travel backwards into the central nervous system), and muscles. It’s difficult to know just what you’re doing to the animal’s nervous system.
Crabs (in this case, Carcinus maenas; pictured) like dark hidey-holes. Magee and Elwood took the crabs, placed them in an open arena with two shelters, one with vertical stripes and one with horizontal stripes. Then, they chose one shelter that if the crab entered it, they gave it an electric shock. They gave the animals 10 trials, which were delivered in short order (two minutes between each trial). The total testing time for each animal seemed to be maybe an hour or two, depending on the crab’s behaviour.
What you would expect is that crabs would learn to avoid the avoid the shelter in which they got shocked. This is indeed what happens (Figure 1 from Magee and Elwood below). But notice that even at the end of the trails, about a third of the crabs are still walking into the shock box. Maybe these are just crab masochists.
The authors went on to test whether it was the stripes on the shelter, or the direction of the shelter, that the crabs had learned by swapping the positions of the two shelters. This seemed to have happened right after the ten trials. Given that part of the argument that they make in the Introduction that “pain facilitates long-term protection because of the ease with which animals learn to avoid that situation and avoid future damage,” and in the Discussion that they are finding “long-term motivational change,” the relatively short time scale is slightly surprising. Magee and Elwood’s argument would be greatly strengthened if they showed the crabs retained the memory after, say, 24 hours or 48 hours. Other studies have shown that decapod crustaceans, like crayfish and lobsters, can remember things, such as who they’ve fought, for days to weeks (reviewed in Hemsworth et al. 2007; see their Table 1).
It’s worth comparing this to an earlier paper that had a similar approach. Kawai et al. (2004) also applied electric shock to crayfish. Those authors had two groups:
- Crayfish shocked while facing a door they could escape through (group F for forward);
- Crayfish shocked while they were facing away from the door (group B for backward).
These authors noted that group F, on the right in the figure below, learned to avoid the shock. But notice that about half the animals in this group never learned the task (S6, 7, and 10), and it took a long time. Group B never improved.
Magee and Elwood mention the long training in the paper by Kawai and colleagues, but not the variation in performance either between the two groups, or within the one group. I wonder if an animal by animal breakdown in the Magee and Elwood paper would show the same sort of individual variation. That is, the Magee and Elwood figure could either be:
- All the crabs getting a little better throughout the trials.
- Some crabs getting very good, while others just never, ever learn.
Now, the crayfish were shocked less than the crabs were. But if the idea being put forward is that avoidance learning suggests pain, would this mean that only about half of crayfish feel pain... and only if they are looking at a door?
Press coverage
Having talked about the paper itself, I want to shift to how it’s being reported. I expected media attention over this paper, because previous papers from the Elwood lab had gotten attention. For instance, a lead interview on Quirks and Quarks. I was still surprised by the breadth of coverage people in my Twitter feed helped me find yesterday. The Daily Mail was the first I found, followed by the BBC, Discovery, The Guardian, National Public Radio, Fox News, Live Science and I’m sure there will be many others.
The Mail article starts off with the old hack, “Scientists have proven...” as though this is a worldwide consensus instead of two people (Magee and Elwood, 2013). Oh, but how such reports grate on me with their use of the word “prove.” (There are exceptions; the BBC puts “further evidence” in its headline, which emphasizes that this is part of a series of studies).
The original paper in JEB is, as often the case, much more nuanced than the press articles suggest. It carefully uses phrases like, “consistent with pain”, which admits there are alternative hypotheses. Elwood and colleagues are doing a series of studies, step by step; the sort of incremental progress that characterizes so much of science.
Let me start by saying that not everyone agrees with the notion generally that crustaceans feel pain. Victoria Braithwaite, in her book Do Fish Feel Pain? (reviewed here), concluded that crustaceans do not feel pain, even after long discussions with Elwood.
The two have very different standards on what they think the criteria for pain should be. Elwood appears to think that to show that an animal feels pain, you only need to show that it responds to a nasty stimulus with something more complicated than a reflex. Braithwaite thinks that to show an animal feels pain, you need to show that animal is capable of consciousness. I think it’s fair to say that neither one is widely accepted as the standard for showing an animal feels pain.
To take a similar case, the evidence for nociception in fish has been clear for a decade now: fish have neurons specialized for tissue damage. Braithwaite gave a book-length argument in Do Feel Feel Pain? why she was convinced fish feel pain. And yet, just weeks ago, a new review article by Rose and colleagues (in press) answered the rhetorical question in its title “Do fish really feel pain?” (my emphasis; I can’t see the title as anything other than a very deliberate snub of Braithwaite’s book) with a definite, “No.”
If people are still engaged in heated debates about pain in fish after ten years, even when the evidence related to the question is much further developed than it is for crustaceans, we are a long way from “proving” crustaceans feel pain.
Following along those lines of whether crustaceans need specific conditions to experience pain, I want to return to the issue of specific stimuli. Here is one of my issues with how this story is being reported:
Electric shock is not boiling water.
Many sources are immediately linking the two things under the common notion of “pain.” But only one was tested in the paper. The Daily Mail wrote:
Chefs may have to think twice before plunging live lobsters into a simmering pot. ...
The finding could have important implications for the food industry, where many chefs boil crabs, lobsters and prawns alive in the belief that they are impervious to pain.
So did Discovery:
If you’ve ever tried to boil a live lobster, you probably noticed that it did not just sit patiently in the pot. Most lobsters put up quite a struggle.
The Guardian also tied this research to cooking:
When it comes to cooking that crab for your dinner, however, it may be best to give it the benefit of the doubt.
So did Cosmos magazine (mirrored at ABC):
A lobster thrown live into boiling water may suffer for many seconds, said a scientist who argues that crustaceans can likely feel pain.
So did Fox News, and added housing into the mix.
Researchers have suspected for some time that live lobsters dunked into boiling water and rubber-banded crustaceans stored in crowded fish market tanks experience tremendous pain.
Even if you agree that electric shock is painful to the crabs, that does not automatically mean that crabs experience pain the same way we do. It’s not a crazy hypothesis, but it is still something to be tested rather than something that has already been demonstrated. For instance, at the recent Society for Integrative and Comparative Biology meeting, one talk showed a short clip rather like this one.
It shows a crab, its claw injured in a fight, crabbing its claw, and ripping it off. You could hear the audience reacting when this was shown; people made the sort of groans that they make when they see the weekly sports blooper reel, particularly when you see accidental hits in the groin area.
Here’s a shorter, (but off-colour!) video of the same behaviour.
This is a reminder that what might seem painful to us is not necessarily painful to a different animal. Tearing off a limb voluntarily is not something a human would do.
Let me give you another quick example.
In humans, capsaicin is the chemical that makes chili peppers hot, and is often used in studies of pain as a painful stimuli. Strictly speaking, the chemical causes sensory neurons called nociceptors to fire. Fruit flies also have nociceptors , so at first blush, it’s perfectly reasonably to think they would avoid food laced with capsaicin.
Nope. The more capsaicin, the more the flies like the food (Al-Anzi et al. 2006). Live Science also earns points in my book for mentioning that naked mole rats do not respond to many stimuli that other mammals avoid and appear painful.
If you want to know if boiling a lobster is painful to said lobster, you should test its response to high temperatures, not electric shock.
It is heartening to see that some of the articles I’ve found obviously used embargo time to find other scientists to comment on the study. Scientists who study fish (Paul Hart, quoted in The Guardian; Lynne Sneddon, quoted by the BBC; Lynne worked on fish nociception, so was a good choice), marine organisms besides decapod crustaceans (Magnus Johnson, also quoted in The Guardian). Only one reported contacted someone who has worked on crustacean behaviour (Francesca Gherhardi, who I just met last week, quoted by Live Science).
Having provided several papers providing evidence that electric shock is painful, I am surprised that NPR reports that Elwood suggests electrocuting crustaceans is a pain-free way of dispatching them.
As for what this might mean for those of us who occasionally dispatch a crustacean or two, the best way to minimize potential pain is likely electrocution or driving a knife through the creature's brain, Elwood says. But as most of us lack specialized machinery and knowledge of crustacean anatomy, the easiest way is still dropping the crab in a pot of boiling water.
I’ve commented on this before (here, here). In crustacean neurobiology labs, the most commonly used anaesthetic for crabs, crayfish, and lobster, is chilling. My suggestion would be to put an animal on crushed ice.
I do not have strong opinions about whether crustaceans feel pain or not. But I do think that this new paper is closer to the beginning of research on this kind of question than it is to the end.
Additional: I find it interesting that The Independent ran this story in their “Food and drink” section.
References
Al-Anzi B, Tracey Jr. WD, Benzer S. 2006. Response of Drosophila to wasabi is mediated by painless, the fly homolog of mammalian TRPA1/ANKTM1. Current Biology 16(10): 1034-1040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.002
Appel M, Elwood RW. 2009. Motivational trade-offs and potential pain experience in hermit crabs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 119: 120-124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.03.013
Braithwaite VA. 2010. Do Fish Feel Pain? Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Elwood RW, Appel M. 2009. Pain experience in hermit crabs? Animal Behaviour 77: 1243-1246. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.01.028
Hemsworth R, Villareal W, Patullo BW, Macmillan DL. 2007. Crustacean social behavioral changes in response to isolation. The Biological Bulletin 213(2): 187-195. http://www.biolbull.org/cgi/content/abstract/213/2/187
Kawai N, Kono R, Sugimoto S. 2004. Avoidance learning in the crayfish (Procambarus clarkii) depends on the predatory imminence of the unconditioned stimulus: a behavior systems approach to learning in invertebrates. Behavioural Brain Research 150(1-2): 229-237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00261-4
Magee B, Elwood RW. 2013. Shock avoidance by discrimination learning in the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) is consistent with a key criterion for pain. The Journal of Experimental Biology 216(3): 353-358. http://dx.doi.org/10.1242/jeb.072041
Rose JD, Arlinghaus R, Cooke SJ, Diggles BK, Sawynok W, Stevens ED, Wynne CDL. 2012. Can fish really feel pain? Fish and Fisheries: in press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/faf.12010
Related posts
Ignorance may not be bliss, but perhaps it is painless
Crustacean nociception: Origins, part 1
Crustacean nociception: Origins, part 2
Crustacean nociception: the worry
Shell shock
Shell shock, revisited
Tenth International Congress for Neuroethology, Day 5 (nociception symposium coverage)
External links
The Daily Mail: Lobsters and crabs can feel pain and take action to avoid it happening again
Live Science: Crabs really do feel pain: Study (Mirrored by Huffington Post: Crabs Feel Pain After All, New Study Suggests)
NPR The Salt: Yes, Virginia, Crabs Likely Feel Pain, But They're Still Delicious
Fox News: Lobsters, crabs feel pain, scientists say
BBC: Further evidence crabs and other crustaceans feel pain
The Guardian: Crab study puts pain on the menu
Cosmos magazine: Be kind to your seafood, study urges
Australian Broadcasting Corporation (ABC) Science: Be kind to your seafood, study urges
The Independent: Shock survey: Lobsters and crabs feel pain
Crab photo by BrentMWilson on Flickr; used under a Creative Commons license.
17 January 2013
Bible classes; vouchers, creationism
The National Center for Science Education reports that creationism is being taught in some Texas K-12 schools in Bible classes.
I am not sure I can get as upset about this as if this were being taught in other courses. It’s a Bible class. What’s on the label is what’s in the tin, so to speak.
Phil Plait also reports on voucher program sused to support sending students to private schools with public money. Texas does not have a voucher program, but is apparently considering it.
I am not sure I can get as upset about this as if this were being taught in other courses. It’s a Bible class. What’s on the label is what’s in the tin, so to speak.
Phil Plait also reports on voucher program sused to support sending students to private schools with public money. Texas does not have a voucher program, but is apparently considering it.
Scientists of Walmart
When you’re travelling, you can pack a lot of experiences into a very small time frame. When I went on my #SciFund expedition last November, I had a lot of moments to think, since I was mostly on my own in the field, either netting crayfish or digging for sand crabs.
The first part of my trip was searching for crayfish, on the edge of the Florida everglades. The place I was collecting had a little boat launch, where small tourism businesses would load up their airboats and take tourists out into Everglades to see birds and alligators and so forth.
I was listening to some of the guys chatting while they were waiting for customers, either to go out or to come back to land. The conversation turned to things like arguing with lawyers and judges over community service sentencing for minor criminal charges (can’t even remember what it was; it was nothing related to the airboat business). I thought, “This is not the sort of talk I normally overhear on my campus.”
It was warm and sunny, and the work was not all that easy, so by the end of my time collecting, I was feeling scruffy. And I had epic hat head.
Near where I was collecting, there was a gas station where I would stop to get a drink. And it was almost the perfect stereotype of rural American culture. Hunting knives. Beer. Fishing. NASCAR.
The kicker:
Somewhere along the way, though, I ended up talking to some other people who had seen me with the nets and asked me what I was doing. On guy in particular said something like, “Crayfish eat just about everything, I guess, and everything else eats them,” which was so close to a line I’d picked up at an Astacology meeting: “Crayfish eat everything, and everything eats crayfish.” He was dressed not too differently than I was; being out in the heat in rural areas doesn’t lend itself to fashion statements. I saw myself, indistinguishable from the other patrons of the gas station, and thought:
I am a scientist of Walmart.
By which I mean this. Some of you may know the People of Walmart site. It’s a window into perspectives on class in America. A lot of the site is about making fun of people who are low class, poorly dressed, rural, or what have you.
And I realized that the kind of science I do, and the kind of scientist I am, could be viewed through the same lens that focuses on the people of Walmart. Here I was out at the edge of the Everglades (i.e., swamp), funded by two little crowdfunding campaigns, trying to answer some small questions about a couple of crustaceans. I work at a rural university. In Texas. I’ve even boasted about my low prices.
What I was doing, – am doing – is not expensive, not fashionable, not sophisticated, either scientifically or culturally. I realized that while I may have the education and I wanted to think that I was above all this gas station culture, no matter how how I worked or what I did, scientifically, I was probably always be viewed as someone mucking around on the fringe of scientific society.
Related posts
We’re having a sale on amazing
Biological costs
A patent clerk’s pay
Hat tip to Scicurious for encouraging me to blog this.
The first part of my trip was searching for crayfish, on the edge of the Florida everglades. The place I was collecting had a little boat launch, where small tourism businesses would load up their airboats and take tourists out into Everglades to see birds and alligators and so forth.
I was listening to some of the guys chatting while they were waiting for customers, either to go out or to come back to land. The conversation turned to things like arguing with lawyers and judges over community service sentencing for minor criminal charges (can’t even remember what it was; it was nothing related to the airboat business). I thought, “This is not the sort of talk I normally overhear on my campus.”
It was warm and sunny, and the work was not all that easy, so by the end of my time collecting, I was feeling scruffy. And I had epic hat head.
Near where I was collecting, there was a gas station where I would stop to get a drink. And it was almost the perfect stereotype of rural American culture. Hunting knives. Beer. Fishing. NASCAR.
The kicker:
Somewhere along the way, though, I ended up talking to some other people who had seen me with the nets and asked me what I was doing. On guy in particular said something like, “Crayfish eat just about everything, I guess, and everything else eats them,” which was so close to a line I’d picked up at an Astacology meeting: “Crayfish eat everything, and everything eats crayfish.” He was dressed not too differently than I was; being out in the heat in rural areas doesn’t lend itself to fashion statements. I saw myself, indistinguishable from the other patrons of the gas station, and thought:
I am a scientist of Walmart.
By which I mean this. Some of you may know the People of Walmart site. It’s a window into perspectives on class in America. A lot of the site is about making fun of people who are low class, poorly dressed, rural, or what have you.
And I realized that the kind of science I do, and the kind of scientist I am, could be viewed through the same lens that focuses on the people of Walmart. Here I was out at the edge of the Everglades (i.e., swamp), funded by two little crowdfunding campaigns, trying to answer some small questions about a couple of crustaceans. I work at a rural university. In Texas. I’ve even boasted about my low prices.
What I was doing, – am doing – is not expensive, not fashionable, not sophisticated, either scientifically or culturally. I realized that while I may have the education and I wanted to think that I was above all this gas station culture, no matter how how I worked or what I did, scientifically, I was probably always be viewed as someone mucking around on the fringe of scientific society.
Related posts
We’re having a sale on amazing
Biological costs
A patent clerk’s pay
Hat tip to Scicurious for encouraging me to blog this.
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)